Examples of How Language Affects Cognition

Most questions of whether and how language shapes thought start with the simple observation that languages differ from one another. And a lot! Let's take a (very) hypothetical example. Suppose you want to say, "Bush read Chomsky's latest book." Let's focus on just the verb, "read." To say this sentence in English, we have to mark the verb for tense; in this case, we have to pronounce it like "red" and not like "reed." In Indonesian you need not (in fact, you can't) alter the verb to mark tense. In Russian you would have to alter the verb to indicate tense and gender. So if it was Laura Bush who did the reading, you'd use a different form of the verb than if it was George. In Russian you'd also have to include in the verb information about completion. If George read only part of the book, you'd use a different form of the verb than if he'd diligently plowed through the whole thing. In Turkish you'd have to include in the verb how you acquired this information: if you had witnessed this unlikely event with your own two eyes, you'd use one verb form, but if you had simply read or heard about it, or inferred it from something Bush said, you'd use a different verb form.

[...]

Believers in cross-linguistic differences counter that everyone does not pay attention to the same things: if everyone did, one might think it would be easy to learn to speak other languages. Unfortunately, learning a new language (especially one not closely related to those you know) is never easy; it seems to require paying attention to a new set of distinctions. Whether it's distinguishing modes of being in Spanish, evidentiality in Turkish, or aspect in Russian, learning to speak these languages requires something more than just learning vocabulary: it requires paying attention to the right things in the world so that you have the correct information to include in what you say.

[...]

Follow me to Pormpuraaw, a small Aboriginal community on the western edge of Cape York, in northern Australia. I came here because of the way the locals, the Kuuk Thaayorre, talk about space. Instead of words like "right," "left," "forward," and "back," which, as commonly used in English, define space relative to an observer, the Kuuk Thaayorre, like many other Aboriginal groups, use cardinal-direction terms — north, south, east, and west — to define space.1 This is done at all scales, which means you have to say things like "There's an ant on your southeast leg" or "Move the cup to the north northwest a little bit." One obvious consequence of speaking such a language is that you have to stay oriented at all times, or else you cannot speak properly. The normal greeting in Kuuk Thaayorre is "Where are you going?" and the answer should be something like " Southsoutheast, in the middle distance." If you don't know which way you're facing, you can't even get past "Hello."

The result is a profound difference in navigational ability and spatial knowledge between speakers of languages that rely primarily on absolute reference frames (like Kuuk Thaayorre) and languages that rely on relative reference frames (like English).2 Simply put, speakers of languages like Kuuk Thaayorre are much better than English speakers at staying oriented and keeping track of where they are, even in unfamiliar landscapes or inside unfamiliar buildings. What enables them — in fact, forces them — to do this is their language. Having their attention trained in this way equips them to perform navigational feats once thought beyond human capabilities. Because space is such a fundamental domain of thought, differences in how people think about space don't end there. People rely on their spatial knowledge to build other, more complex, more abstract representations. Representations of such things as time, number, musical pitch, kinship relations, morality, and emotions have been shown to depend on how we think about space. So if the Kuuk Thaayorre think differently about space, do they also think differently about other things, like time? This is what my collaborator Alice Gaby and I came to Pormpuraaw to find out.

To test this idea, we gave people sets of pictures that showed some kind of temporal progression (e.g., pictures of a man aging, or a crocodile growing, or a banana being eaten). Their job was to arrange the shuffled photos on the ground to show the correct temporal order. We tested each person in two separate sittings, each time facing in a different cardinal direction. If you ask English speakers to do this, they'll arrange the cards so that time proceeds from left to right. Hebrew speakers will tend to lay out the cards from right to left, showing that writing direction in a language plays a role.3 So what about folks like the Kuuk Thaayorre, who don't use words like "left" and "right"? What will they do?

The Kuuk Thaayorre did not arrange the cards more often from left to right than from right to left, nor more toward or away from the body. But their arrangements were not random: there was a pattern, just a different one from that of English speakers. Instead of arranging time from left to right, they arranged it from east to west. That is, when they were seated facing south, the cards went left to right. When they faced north, the cards went from right to left. When they faced east, the cards came toward the body and so on. This was true even though we never told any of our subjects which direction they faced. The Kuuk Thaayorre not only knew that already (usually much better than I did), but they also spontaneously used this spatial orientation to construct their representations of time.

[...]

An important question at this point is: Are these differences caused by language per se or by some other aspect of culture? Of course, the lives of English, Mandarin, Greek, Spanish, and Kuuk Thaayorre speakers differ in a myriad of ways. How do we know that it is language itself that creates these differences in thought and not some other aspect of their respective cultures?

One way to answer this question is to teach people new ways of talking and see if that changes the way they think. In our lab, we've taught English speakers different ways of talking about time. In one such study, English speakers were taught to use size metaphors (as in Greek) to describe duration (e.g., a movie is larger than a sneeze), or vertical metaphors (as in Mandarin) to describe event order. Once the English speakers had learned to talk about time in these new ways, their cognitive performance began to resemble that of Greek or Mandarin speakers. This suggests that patterns in a language can indeed play a causal role in constructing how we think.6 In practical terms, it means that when you're learning a new language, you're not simply learning a new way of talking, you are also inadvertently learning a new way of thinking. Beyond abstract or complex domains of thought like space and time, languages also meddle in basic aspects of visual perception — our ability to distinguish colors, for example. Different languages divide up the color continuum differently: some make many more distinctions between colors than others, and the boundaries often don't line up across languages.

Notes:

Examples of how languages differ between cultures in their constructs, how those constructs affect the way the speaker thinks about things, and how teaching a person a new language can alter the way they think.

Folksonomies: culture cognition language

Keywords:
Kuuk Thaayorre (0.913152 (negative:-0.071643)), English speakers (0.802347 (positive:0.274689)), new language (0.682577 (neutral:0.000000)), different verb form (0.661017 (neutral:0.000000)), way (0.627196 (positive:0.323550)), different form (0.625582 (negative:-0.390501)), Kuuk Thaayorre speakers (0.624405 (neutral:0.000000)), spatial knowledge (0.624386 (positive:0.484349)), time (0.622810 (negative:-0.095833)), languages (0.618887 (positive:0.431699)), small Aboriginal community (0.617414 (positive:0.317893)), reference frames (0.613834 (positive:0.589326)), collaborator Alice Gaby (0.613521 (neutral:0.000000)), absolute reference frames (0.612131 (positive:0.589326)), relative reference frames (0.611366 (neutral:0.000000)), progression (e.g., pictures of a man aging, or a crocodile growing, or a banana being eaten). (0.609411 (neutral:0.000000)), different cardinal direction (0.609159 (neutral:0.000000)), correct temporal order (0.606469 (neutral:0.000000)), verb information (0.603454 (neutral:0.000000)), space (0.599418 (negative:-0.003924)), new ways (0.598545 (positive:0.827220)), things (0.598085 (positive:0.205820)), new way (0.596693 (negative:-0.311672)), right things (0.573623 (positive:0.623147)), simple observation (0.573294 (positive:0.567314)), latest book (0.572615 (negative:-0.729635)), hypothetical example (0.571996 (negative:-0.330342)), Cognition Examples (0.570753 (neutral:0.000000)), new set (0.570636 (neutral:0.000000)), cross-linguistic differences (0.567928 (negative:-0.230250))

Entities:
Kuuk Thaayorre:GeographicFeature (0.914071 (positive:0.077793)), Laura Bush:Person (0.501738 (negative:-0.532580))

Concepts:
Space (0.979326): dbpedia | freebase
Time (0.828957): dbpedia | freebase | opencyc
Spacetime (0.801900): dbpedia | freebase | yago
Left-wing politics (0.720792): dbpedia | freebase | opencyc
Frame of reference (0.705706): dbpedia | freebase | opencyc
Political spectrum (0.697384): dbpedia | freebase | yago
French Revolution (0.692248): dbpedia | freebase
Right-wing politics (0.670411): dbpedia | freebase | opencyc

 How Does Our Language Shape the Way We Think?
Electronic/World Wide Web>Internet Article:  Boroditsky, Lera (6.12.09), How Does Our Language Shape the Way We Think?, Edge, Retrieved on 2013-04-26
  • Source Material [www.edge.org]
  • Folksonomies: culture cognition language